Saturday, December 16, 2017

#21 singing and tenure, measuring wisdom, seats in the U.S. capitol, customers of a clothing store, what I learned on the job, condolence term




Good morning!

            Inasmuch as I'm no longer sending out an annual tome, I want to take this occasion to wish everyone a convivial and upbeat end of the year and a happy and healthy 2018!  I hope your lives, and those of your families, get better and better.

* * *

            Early in the year Kathy and I went to two operas the same day.  Not our choice, but you go when the Met Opera is doing a simulcast in the afternoon and you go when you have Minnesota Opera tickets that night that you can't change because you're out of town for all the other dates.  So we were stuck with going to two.  That won't happen again.

            Neither of us was particularly looking forward to the evening opera, but we went.  Diana's Garden turned out to be a delightful piece, funny and lively.  I noticed that the lead soprano, Leah Partridge, was a professor of music at a university in Georgia, so I looked her up on the institutional website.  I learned on the site that she was an assistant professor of voice, which means she could have had a probationary appointment, tenure-track (but not necessarily—it could have been some kind of adjunct appointment not on the tenure track).  I was curious about whether a performance engagement like that in Minnesota—where she had to be for a week—contributed to positive reviews for tenure.  I emailed her (at her institutional email address) to ask and told her I'd worked with the U of Minnesota tenure code off and on for 40 years.  Of course I told her we very much liked the opera and her performance, both of which were true.

            Interestingly, she emailed back to me fairly promptly, said my email could not have come at a more opportune moment in her life, told me she was indeed on a tenure-track appointment, and asked if she could call me about tenure.  That was a little surprising, but I said "sure."  She did the next day, and we talked for 45 minutes, mostly about the tenure and promotion process, what she should be looking for, documents to review, her particular circumstances, etc.  It turns out she was in year 4 of the probationary period, with either one or two to go, and had questions about events occurring in her school.  (For those of you outside higher education, the standard probationary period for faculty members is six years, and then it's either up—tenure is granted—or it's out—please go work somewhere else.  There are a few exceptions to the six-year rule, such as in medicine and some business schools, but the overwhelming majority of American faculty members in a tenure-track/tenure system have a six-year probationary period.)

Yes, performances do count to tenure in music, although the question of how many does arise.  Anyway, she told me I'd been very helpful and told me she'd keep me posted.

            I closed the conversation by reverting to opera.  I asked her whether a claim I have made in the past is accurate:  I maintain that opera singers have by far the best trained voices in the world (or at least there are none better).  Now, I recognize I was asking someone with a bias, but she said I was right and that there are reasons, such as learning control, volume, and consistency.  She said that very few popular, rock, gospel, and other singers receive voice training, or if they do, it's very little compared to opera singers.  I told her I'd purchased a Christmas CD a number of years ago that had Diana Ross and Placido Domingo and Luciano Pavarotti singing Christmas songs—and that Ross sounded thin and tinny compared to the two tenors.  But Ross, singing with the Supremes, had never sounded bad to me.  Ms. Partridge told me it's because of lack of training.  So I got my money's worth out of the conversation by having a long-standing opinion confirmed by a biased but knowledgeable source!

* * *

            Can you measure wisdom?  A mitt full of psychiatrists at the University of California, San Diego (2017) have developed a "new scale for assessing wisdom based on common domains and a neurobiological model."  It had never occurred to me that there are psychometric measures for wisdom, but I came to realize that there is indeed a significant psychological research literature on wisdom that dates back several decades.  There are nine scales in existence, and this new one, the authors report, correlates well with the older tests.  International researchers agree on the elements of wisdom and those elements appear in the Bhagavad Gita as well as in books of the Bible and other religious documents.

            When I began to read about measurement of wisdom, I thought of Oscar Wilde:  “With age comes wisdom, but sometimes age comes alone.”  I also thought of that proverb I've heard often, "there's no fool like an old fool" (which apparently dates from the middle of the 16th century).  General social consensus seems to be that you ought to get wiser as you get older.

            So what's wisdom?  The idea has been around since the dawn of human civilization, but only recently has there been a systematic and scientific attempt at definition.  It's seen as important for "general physical and mental health, well-being, happiness, life satisfaction, personal mastery, and resilience" [citations omitted].

            Here's one definition from a researcher who has studied wisdom for a long time, Jeffrey Dean Webster:

Wisdom is the competence in, intention to, and application of, critical life experiences to facilitate the optimal development of self and others. 

[So,] it is understood that a wise person possesses at least a minimal level of competence in decision-making, problem-solving, and other forms of intellectual abilities. . . .  Wise persons are also “mindful” in that they deliberately intend their actions to have wisdom-related consequences. That is, wisdom is not an accidental by-product of some process. . . .  Intentions to act wisely are nothing but unfulfilled potential unless they are acted upon.  Application, therefore, is required if potentially wise behaviors are to be more than unrealized “New Year’s Eve” resolutions.  Wisdom is forged in the exigencies of real life and emerges from grappling with critical life events, not the mundane, trivial minutiae of daily life.  Reflections upon such key occurrences enable wise individuals to set goals in multiple life endeavors (e.g., personal, career, health, leisure, artistic, spiritual) which contribute to optimal growth, that is, the realization of full potential. . . .  Finally, wise individuals are not sage misers, hoarding their hard earned secrets of successful life and fulfillment; rather, they seek to engage others and share these valuable insights within their broader community.

He identified five aspects of wisdom.

Openness:  rigid and inflexible responses to life’s demands mark an individual as unwise. . . .  Emotional Regulation:  affect sensitivity or emotional regulation are key elements of wisdom. . . .  Humor:  the wise individual recognizes, enjoys, and uses humor in a variety of contexts and for myriad purposes. “I can’t imagine a wise old person who can’t laugh. The world is full of ridiculous dichotomies. . . .   Critical Life Experience:  the crucible of wisdom consists of the turmoil and uncertainty inherent in many important life decisions [that are] morally ambiguous, multifaceted, and fraught with unknown outcomes [that] pressure individuals to make choices under suboptimal conditions [such as end-of-life decisions, career choices, and relationship issues]. . . .  Reminiscence and Reflectiveness: critical life events, in and of themselves, do not necessarily contribute to wisdom unless we reflect upon them [so] we are better prepared to confront similar issues in the future (emphasis added).

            The UC San Diego group, drawing on the foregoing as well as the broader literature, concluded that at least in western society, empirically-based wisdom comprises (the following is a quote):

(1) general knowledge of life and social decision making - ability to give good advice, life knowledge, and life skills;
(2) emotional regulation - affect regulation and self-control;
(3) pro-social behaviors - e.g., empathy, compassion, altruism, and a sense of fairness;
(4) insight - the ability and desire to understand oneself and one's actions at a deep level;
(5) value relativism (tolerance for divergent values) - being nonjudgmental and accepting of other value systems; and
(6) decisiveness - the ability to make quick and effective decisions.

These attributes, the research suggests, come together in a way that is greater than any one of them alone in constituting wisdom—the whole is greater than the sum of the parts.

            Personally—and without any deep dive into the scholarship—I'd go with Webster on including humor.  Someone who's wise but never laughs is a bore.  I'm not sure I agree with Webster on the claim that wisdom is gained in the caldron of difficult life events.  It seems to me that people can learn wisdom in the course of daily life, one's own and observation of others'.  Traumatic or ambiguous events may indeed contribute to wisdom, but it seems to be a stretch to say they are the primary or only source.

            The San Diego group proposes an interesting set of factors.  I'd certainly concede that anyone who had all six of them to a significant degree would be someone I'd consider "wise."  Most of us—speaking for myself—probably have a mix of them, stronger on some and less strong on others.  As I do a mental survey of my good friends, I conclude that they are all wise in varying degrees—but, beyond doubt, wise.

            I contemplate my two children.  Even though I would give almost anything to have her back, I must concede that Krystin was not especially wise except perhaps in the last couple of years.  She was smart, but not wise for most of her life; where she failed earlier was on (1), (4), and (6).  It does seem to me that strength in (1) can really only come with age and experience, and Krystin had barely reached the point where she had the "ability to give good advice, life knowledge, and life skills."  That's not really a criticism of Krystin; I'd argue there are few among us who would be strong on (1) at a young age.  On the other hand, Krystin was extraordinarily strong on (2), (3), and (5).  The tributes to her on her Facebook page and in the cards I received attest to those.  (Given her years of medical travails and pain, her strength on (2) is nothing short of astounding.)

            Conversely, in many ways Elliott has been wise beyond his years for much of his life.  (I do not claim he had the wisdom of an adult when he was, for example, 6 or 12 years old, only that he seems to always have been wiser than his age would suggest likely.)  Oddly, his strengths are Krystin's weaknesses; he has been exceptionally strong on (4) and (6) and less so on (2) and (3).  Contrary to my assertion in the previous paragraph about wisdom coming with age on (1), I think Elliott's actually strong on (1) even at age 27.

            And what about me?  Self-assessments are always tricky and often wrong.  But I'll do one anyway.  As with most people, I suggested, I'm better on (1) than I was 30 years ago.  I'm pretty good on (2).  I've gotten better at (3), especially empathy and compassion, as I've gotten older; I was lousy at them for much of my life.  (4) I'm probably good at this one (although, as I think about it, this strikes me as another element that might come with age and experience, and I have no doubt I've gotten better at this the farther along in life I've gotten).  I'm decent on (5), although increasingly I have trouble accepting values that I regard as outright nonsense (such as anti-science views and intolerant views on the part of others).  If you don't count the bridge table, I'm probably average on (6).  At the bridge table, my skill on this score declines with each passing year.

            I am curious to know if those of you who have known me for years think I'm even close to the mark in my self-assessment.

            I'm not so dumb that I'm going to write an assessment of Kathy's strengths and weaknesses.

So with those examples, I support my own hypothesis:  we have a mix of strengths and weaknesses.

            If I'm accurate in my analysis of the two children, what a puzzle.  How could two children, raised in the same household in exactly the same way (as much as two kids can be raised in the same way), turn out so dramatically differently on these measures?  I can only conclude that genetics played some significant role.  Elliott inherited the calm and thoughtful characters of his grandfathers; Krystin did not.

            The UC San Diego psychiatrists' study was focused on linking the attributes of wisdom to specific parts of the brain to "form the basis of a putative neurocircuitry model of wisdom."  Their work, detailed at length in the article, was successful on five of the six elements of wisdom, but not on (1).  I'm not surprised they didn't find any relationship to brain circuitry and (1) because (1) is a much more generalized understanding of life than the other five elements.  (As good researchers, they also point out the shortcomings of their findings, including the fact that they had a disproportionate number of people with higher education and more Caucasians than are in the general population.)

            I find myself with two questions (off the top of my head) about this research.  One of them is whether there is a link between intelligence and wisdom (granting, as I always do, that we don't have good measures or definitions of intelligence).  If you weren't born with much brain power to start with, how wise can you get?  For some, it may be impossible *not* to be an old fool—through no fault of their own.

The other question is whether there's a missing element in the components of the definition in addition to humor.  I give them full credit for careful effort to draw on the research in identifying the six components that they do, but I wonder if the ability to delay gratification shouldn't fit in there somewhere.  I don't see how it falls readily into any of the six that they list, but surely that's an element of wisdom.  It's what Krystin couldn't do and Elliott can.

While writing this bit on measuring wisdom, I happened on this news article on Pacific Standard:

University of Wisconsin–Madison psychology professor Seth Pollak worked with over 50 people around the age of 20, and found that those who had experienced extreme stress as kids were hampered in their ability to make good decisions as adults.  Simply put, childhood trauma—due to circumstances like neglect or exposure to violence—created young adults fundamentally unable to correctly consider risk and make healthy life decisions [https://psmag.com/social-justice/childhood-trauma-adversely-affects-decision-making]

One tiny example of why wisdom may not come with age and, again, the individual's not at fault.

            It should be evident, given the two different lists of factors from the 2010s, that wisdom research isn't a settled field, to say the least.  In an annual review article about the research on wisdom in 2011, the authors identified a similar but not identical set of attributes of wisdom:

The cognitive-ability component combines crystallized and fluid aspects of intelligence:  Both an experience-based body of broad and deep life knowledge and the ability to reason well and think logically about novel problems are associated with wisdom, although the former aspect is viewed as more central.  The second component, searching for insight, bridges cognition and motivation:  Wise individuals are willing and able to understand complex issues deeply rather than superficially. If they are lacking sufficient information, they will search for that information rather than form a premature judgment.  Third, a related, more motivational-emotional component is wise people's reflective attitude: Rather than making quick judgments or being guided by strong emotions, they prefer to think deeply about people, the world, and themselves.  Their attitude of looking at all sides of an issue also implies a willingness to be critical of themselves, a balanced manner of regulating their own emotions rather than getting carried away by strong feelings, and an unobtrusive self-presentation.  Fourth, wise people also tend to show high levels of concern for others:  In addition to being cognitively able to see others' perspectives, they transcend their self-interests and care deeply for the well-being of others.  Because this attitude goes beyond one's family and close friends, wise people often engage in civic activities for the benefit of others.  These four components manifest themselves in concrete activity rather than only in theory:  Wise individuals are assumed to have real-world problem-solving skills that enable them to apply their knowledge and judgment to concrete problems faced by themselves and others (emphasis added).

They also observe (a la Wilde) that "Wisdom was also one of only two positive characteristics that laypeople viewed both as positive and as specific to old age.  On the other hand, however, older age is viewed as neither necessary nor sufficient for wisdom:  Most people are aware that not everyone develops wisdom with old age, and that young people can also be quite wise."

* * *

            In response to my story about visiting the Minnesota state capitol building, my friend Rick Evans told me a story.  (Rick is the one who gave us the tour; his job takes him to the capitol quite a lot.)  Years ago, Rick was chief of staff to Minnesota Senator David Durenberger.  He wrote to me recalling his experience.

Speaking of Capitol buildings, when I was in Washington I loved to give tours of the U.S. Capitol.  I also took every tour that was ever offered by anyone because I could learn from every tour.  One fun fact I used to feature on my tours of the U.S. Capitol: 

Unlike the Minnesota Capitol the U.S. Capitol has the House and one end and the Senate at the other.  These were not the original chambers of the houses.  If you tour the U.S. Capitol, and are standing in Statuary Hall, you are actually standing in the original House Chamber.  The original Senate Chamber is preserved as it was and is used for ceremonial occasions.  In the 1850’s, due to the increasing numbers of states, the new chambers were planned and constructed.  The House is on the south end of the Capitol and Speaker’s seat faces North.  The Senate is on the North end of the Capitol and the President of the Senate faces South.  When all the doors in between are opened – as they are, more often than you would think – it is possible for the President of the Senate and the Speaker to look eye-to-eye down the entire length of the Capitol.  The chairs of the respective officers are at the exact same height.  While some people (and Wikipedia) refer to the House and Senate as the Lower House and the Upper House, respectively, I was told on one of the tours that I took with the Capitol Historical Society that the equal height of the chairs was intentional and symbolic of the fact that the U.S. does not have an upper chamber and a lower chamber.

Rick gave us one of the U.S. capitol tours when he was in Durenberger's office and we were visiting Washington, D.C.  We got to go and see places no one on a normal tour gets to go and see.  It was great!

* * *

            Another friend, Professor emeritus John Adams, was reminded of a story when he read about Julie Belle White and her father's clothing store.

Your item on Hubert H. White reminded me of the hundreds of times I passed that shop on Marquette beginning in the 1940s, through high school on the streetcars and buses. 

I wondered about its clientele, and one time when I was downtown after a meeting at city hall, I decided to stop on and look over the place.  It was about 2:30 p.m. on a weekday in the fall around 1985.  I was dressed in my U of M professorial tweed jacket, with shirt and tie.

As I walked in, there were no customers visible but there were three sales guys chatting and leaning against counters.  They looked me over, none of them moving.  I assumed from my appearance they knew I wasn't a good prospect.  As I approached them they greeted me and asked if they could help me.  I replied, "I'm a U of M professor and have lived in Minneapolis almost all of my life.  I've passed your shop hundreds of times and the same question always occurs to me, and today I had a minute to stop in and ask.

"Who are your customers?" I asked.  The fellows looked at one another. There was a silence of about 15 seconds.  Then one of them offered, "We provide clothing to men who sell things that you can't see."

It was an excellent answer.  I've thought about it many times since.

* * *

            It occurred to me recently that the responsibilities of my job for 28 years led to a (very modest) education in a wide variety of fields.  Among other things, I worked closely with faculty members who served as committee chairs.  While most of our interactions were focused on committee and university business, we inevitably also talked about their work.  At a place like the University of Minnesota (as with other similar colleges and universities around the world), these faculty members are among the leading experts in their field; they both teach and do research in it.  I was always nosy enough to ask what they were spending their academic time on, and they were patient enough to explain their work to me.  In some cases, they had to talk to me like I was a first-year student.

            I thus had the benefit learning from faculty members in (to name only some of the fields) mechanical engineering, horticulture, public health, physics, political science, law, dance, physical therapy, chemistry, vet medicine, computer science, forestry, plant biology, psychology, information and decision science, sociology, aerospace engineering, geography—the list could go on.  I'm not really going anywhere with this point, other than to muse that it was a superb way to get a really, really, truly liberal—even if superficial—education! 

* * *

            The use of a word struck me in the last few weeks.  Many people were kind enough to send me touching and thoughtful cards and messages after Krystin's death.  I am grateful for every one of them.  A fair number who contacted me used the word "unbearable" in their condolences.  Well, no.

            I am aware, from long personal experience, that one struggles to find words of compassion and sympathy when a friend has had to deal with the death of a family member.  It occurred to me, however, that the term "unbearable" standing alone isn't right.  If the death of a loved one were truly unbearable, you'd be contemplating suicide.  That act is not part of my life plans (and if it were, Krystin would be horrified).  I am *not* being critical of those who used the term; I've probably used it myself in the past.  But as I say, it dawned on me that however much you wish to convey support and sharing in grief, the word "unbearable" by itself isn't the one you ought to use.

            "Nearly unbearable" or "almost unbearable" would work (and, in many cases, would probably be about as accurate—and poignant—as you could be).

            There's my entirely-made-up etiquette lesson for the day.



No comments:

Post a Comment

Most Read