Tuesday, September 19, 2017

#8 interests; ketchup & rheology; food bits; conservatism; pain scales; income & socializing; cussing; 1931 house cost





            It has always been a mystery to me how and why people develop the interests in life that they do.  I mentioned this puzzle a few years ago.  Further evidence of my lack of understanding emerged this summer.  In the process of decluttering, I brought up from the basement two very large brandy snifters (not really for brandy, more for filling with shells or marbles or whatever, probably two-gallon capacity or more).  I intended to get rid of them. 

            Elliott eyed the snifters and asked what I was going to do with them.  I said probably donate them to Goodwill.  He promptly took possession and said he wanted to create terrariums.  I thought that a perfectly fine idea, so we went to Bachman's and got the plants and charcoal and pebbles and soil.  Every day during the weeks after which he planted them, Elliott kept an eye on them like a parent keeps an eye on a child. 

A week later he bought 3 more glass containers and a couple of days later we had to visit Bachman's again.  Somehow I created a monster just by trying to get rid of stuff in the basement.  This venture is from someone who has never once shown any interest in gardening or plants of any kind.  Who knows?

* * *

            OK, I admit this falls in the category of slightly goofy, but I read a short research summary that addresses a problem universal to those in the industrialized world.  Perhaps you have heard or read Richard Armour's famous little poem (often attributed to Ogden Nash):

Shake and shake the catsup bottle.

None will come, and then a lot'll.


It turns out that there is a research field, rheology, "the branch of physics concerned with the flow and change of shape of matter," that includes ketchup.  A faculty friend of mine in Mechanical Engineering explained some of this.

Rheology, essentially the flow of thick materials, is a branch of fluid mechanics, so right in the domain of mechanical engineering.  But, rheology also covers flow of mud and silt so also in the sweet spot of some branches of civil engineering.  And, rheology also covers plate tectonics so in the sweet spot of some geologists.  But, [it is] also about flow of some plastics so naturally falls in the sweet spot of some chemical engineers.  Safest to say it is a branch of fluid mechanics.

Another phrase for the materials covered by rheology is non-Newtonian fluids.  The reason is that fluids whose properties are fixed were studied by Newton and are therefore called Newtonian fluids.  Newton did not study ketchup (as far as we know), which is a fluid whose flow properties change when you push on it, and therefore is a non-Newtonian fluid.

I had to do a little exploring to figure this out, and found a wonderful explanation.

You have to pull the trigger on a water pistol to get the water to squirt out. To make the water to come out faster, you have to pull the trigger harder. Fluids resist flow. This phenomenon is known as viscosity.

Newton devised a simple model for fluid flow that could be used to relate how hard you have to pull the trigger to how fast the liquid will squirt out of the pistol. . . .  Fluids like water and gasoline behave according to Newton's model, and are called Newtonian fluids.

But ketchup, blood, yogurt, gravy, pie fillings, mud, and cornstarch paste DON'T follow the model. They're non-Newtonian fluids because doubling the speed that the layers slide past each other does not double the resisting force. It may less than double (like ketchup), or it may more than double (as in the case of quicksand and gravy). That's why stirring gravy thickens it, and why struggling in quicksand will make it even harder to escape.

For some fluids (like mud, or snow) you can push and get no flow at all—until you push hard enough, and the substance begins to flow like a normal liquid. This is what causes mudslides and avalanches.

The phenomenon is known as suspension viscosity.  It holds the material together at no or low force, "but once it yields to a force and starts to flow, the viscosity decreases the faster it flows. . . .  At very high velocities, the suspension viscosity can be similar to water."  Hence, "then a lot'll"!

Ketchup, one learns, "prefers to be in the bottle because it is technically a solid, not a liquid."  Water flows out of the bottle because it is liquid; ketchup does not.  Ketchup is a bunch of tiny pieces of tomato that touch and create a substance that is a solid, which resists movement.  As a solid, it only moves when force is applied, which is why we have to whack the bottle to get the ketchup out.  (The scientists at Heinz have determined that the optimal flow speed for ketchup is 0.028 MPH.)  To get the optimal amount of force, I learned that one tips the bottle at a 45-degree angle and taps it until the ketchup begins to flow.  Science verifying everyday experience!

            It's amusing that ketchup has been a focus of rheological research, but as you can imagine, there are a multitude of applications of this field that affect our daily lives in ways far more important than the flow of ketchup, such as in "paint and industrial applications like sewage treatment and mining."

            (Armour also penned a similar piece about that other great condiment:

Nothing attracts / the mustard from wieners /
as much as the slacks / just back from the cleaners.)

* * *

            While on the subject of food, three unimportant observations.

1.  I have finally decided, after 6½ decades of life, that I really do not like the flavor of thyme, except perhaps in microscopic amounts.  I made a few dishes during the year with thyme in them and realized I didn't like it.  My point here is not so much my particular taste in spices, it's that I find it amusing that it took me six decades to learn something that is a rather mundane part of life.  The encouraging part of the story is that I can still learn.

2.  I find it funny that containers of table salt carry an expiration date.  What I can learn from apparently reputable sources on the web confirms my common-sense understanding:  salt does not expire.  For pete's sake, it sits in the earth for thousands of years.  (Of course, if it gets damp or is contaminated with other material, it may become unusable, but kept dry and sealed, it lasts forever.)

3.  It really is true that science does not care what you believe; the laws of physics will operate irrespective of what you think.  I proved this early in the year.  I had a large refrigerated crockpot full of leftover chicken stew.  I transferred the leftovers to a smaller container and put the crockpot in the sink to soak before cleaning it.  I ran warm water in it.  Within a minute I heard a "crack" and the crockpot had a neat break line right through the middle.  I know that hot water on cold glass will break it, but I learned that even warm water on cold glass does it in.  I really wanted to go buy another crockpot.  Said Gary never.

            My mother-in-law shortly thereafter gave me a new crockpot.  In keeping with the times, of course, it has digital controls, unlike my old one, which had a knob one turned to "low," "high," "keep warm," or "off."

* * *

There are parts of a conservative political philosophy that I can respect (even while disagreeing) and parts that I can even agree with.  Washington Post columnist Michael Gerson captured some of my own sentiments in an opinion piece last December (2016).  He began by bemoaning the fact that conservatism, in his view, is "at its nadir just as the Republican Party is at its zenith."  He thus immediately split the Republican party from conservatism.  A wise judgment.

To the extent Gerson accurately defines conservatism, I don't buy all of it—but parts that I might or might not endorse depend on definitions.  For instance, he maintains that conservatism favors limited government.  What's "limited"?  Does it include national health insurance?  Free public education through college?  Mandatory parental leave for child birth or adoption?  Mandatory wage levels?  If it does, then I favor limited government, too.  I suspect, however, that several of those are transpontine for conservatives.

On the matter of human nature, Gerson argues that "conservatives believe that human beings are fallible and prone to ambition, passion and selfishness. They (actually, we) tend to become swaggering dictators in realms where we can act with impunity—a motor vehicle department office, a hostile traffic stop, a country under personal rule."  What conservatives maybe, and Republicans certainly, fail to recognize is that those same characteristics—ambition, passion and selfishness [sic, no Oxford comma]—inhere in the people who lead the corporate world as well as the political.  In the corporate arena, however, government constraints—anti-trust, pollution, tax, etc.—are vilified by the political right.  I think conservatives aren't always certain about how to contend with rampant private sector abuses that harm people as well as the body politic.

"Conservatives believe that finite and fallen creatures are often wrong. . . . All of us have things to learn, even from our political opponents. The truth is out there, but it is generally broken into pieces and scattered across the human experience. We only reassemble it through listening and civil communication."  The term "fallen" carries a religious connotation that I can't accept, but the general sentiment is one that's hard to disagree with.  As Madison famously wrote in the Federalist #51, "If men were angels, no government would be necessary.  If angels were to govern men, neither external nor internal controls on government would be necessary."
"And conservatives believe that a just society depends on the moral striving of finite and fallen creatures who treat each other with a respect and decency that laws can encourage but not enforce."  I'm not sure that it's accurate to say that laws cannot enforce respect and decency.  It can be argued that behavior follows the law; when hate speech is outlawed, people will be less likely to indulge in it.  Laws against segregation in public facilities is but one example.
"In the midst of all our justified skepticism, we can never be skeptical of this:  that the reason for politics is to honor the equal value of every life, beginning with the weakest and most vulnerable."   If that's true, then conservatives ought to be vigorous supporters of early childhood education and the availability of health care for parents and children, especially the latter.
            Gerson uses lofty language, but as one friend of mine commented, "he seems to be describing a conservatism that is both laudable and absent in most conservatives I have met."  Another wrote that while Gerson's views are ones he "can understand and often support, . . .  apparently those folks have no place in the new Republican Party.  Rapacious capitalists and Top Brass seem to be the model GOP leaders of the moment."  At the time I composed these paragraphs, there appeared an article (which I couldn't find later) articulating the same thought:  the plans and policies of the current leadership in GOP Washington have little or nothing to do with a thoughtful conservative's view of what a government should do (or not).

            Conservatives, it seems to me, ignore the sentence in Madison's Federalist that immediately precedes the more famous one:  "But what is government itself, but the greatest of all reflections on human nature?"  When one contemplates human nature as exemplified in social organizations, the need for government is immediately apparent.  Ronald Reagan really got things backward when he declared that government is the problem, not the solution.  In light of what people in the sugar, tobacco, oil, and auto industries alone have done over the last 50 years, one trembles at the thought that they would have more freedom to do what they choose.  The health care and drug industry record has a few pock marks in it as well.  Human nature, embodied in corporate behavior, for example, indeed needs government.  While governments can do bad, and even evil (it isn't hard to think of instances in American history—Native Americans, for example—much less history from around the world—Nazi Germany, for example), given a stable civil government like the U.S. has had for the decades since World War II, the evil it commits seems to me far less than the evil that has been attempted by the private sector.  So also for France, Germany, Britain, Australia, Japan, etc.  The idea of government protecting its citizens from themselves seems to be lost on conservativism.

            I'm really not, at heart, all that radical a human being, at least in my life and habits.  No surprise there, I'm sure, to anyone who knows me.  I don't want a revolution because revolutions have a way of eating their offspring and doing a lot of harm besides.  It would not be good for my children and it wouldn't likely be much good for most children.  But the way "conservatism" has been embodied in American politics pushes me hard to the left.  If there really were a compassionate conservatism, it might be something I could live with.  But it may fall in the same category as unicorns.


* * *
           
When Krystin was in the hospital at one point, the attending physician asked her what her pain level was—and used various adjectives.  I expressed surprise that he didn't use the ubiquitous 1-10 scale that I've seen in every doctor's office for at least a couple of decades.  He explained that at this (the University) hospital, at least, they no longer used that scale.  They discovered that one person's "3" on the scale might be another person's "8"; one person might say pain was an excruciating 9 but they could continue to function while another might say their pain was a 3—but they were completely disabled from doing anything.  So instead they now try to use different adjectives.

            A British writer by the name of John Henry Walsh wrote a piece about the measurement (or lack thereof) of pain.  After relating a story about a prolonged painful medical experience his wife had gone through, he said that

several questions came into my head.  Chief among them was:  Can anyone in the medical profession talk about pain with any authority?  From the family doctor to the surgeon, their remarks and suggestions seemed tentative, generalized, unknowing — and potentially dangerous:  Was it right for the doctor to tell my wife that her level of pain didn’t sound like appendicitis when the doctor didn’t know whether she had a high or low pain threshold?  Should he have advised her to stay in bed and risk her appendix exploding into peritonitis?  How could surgeons predict that patients would feel only ‘discomfort’ after such an operation when she felt agony — an agony that was aggravated by fear that the operation had been a failure?"

I can personally relate to the last sentence.  When I had spinal column surgery in 1992 (to remove a calcium build up that was pinching nerves), the physician told me that taking a bone spur from my hip (to replace the disk they would need to remove to get at the calcium build up) would cause "some discomfort."  "Some discomfort" indeed.  Rarely has a two-word phrase in English been so abused.  I've never felt such agonizing pain as when I moved my leg after the surgery; doing so sent spasms of teeth-gritting pain up and down my entire body that caused me involuntarily to cry out.  Obviously I've never experienced childbirth, but from the descriptions I've heard, my post-op pain must have come close.

            Walsh went on to wonder "if there were any agreed words that would help a doctor understand the pain felt by a patient."  He recalled his father, a physician, describing the range of pain descriptors his patients had used.  "Few of them, he told me, corresponded to the symptoms listed in a medical textbook.  So how should he proceed? By guesswork and aspirin?"

There seemed to be a chasm of understanding in human discussions of pain.  I wanted to find out how the medical profession apprehends pain – the language it uses for something that’s invisible to the naked eye, that can’t be measured except by asking for the sufferer’s subjective description, and that can be treated only by the use of opium derivatives that go back to the Middle Ages.

            Walsh described the procedure that clinics use, called the McGill Pain Questionnaire.   "The patient listens as a list of ‘pain descriptors’ is read out and has to say whether each word describes their pain — and, if so, to rate the intensity of the feeling. The clinicians then look at the questionnaire and put check marks in the appropriate places.  This gives the clinician a number, or a percentage figure, to work with in assessing, later, whether a treatment has brought the patient’s pain down (or up)."  This isn't an approach I've ever encountered in a doctor visit.

Walsh also notes that "a more recent variant is the National Initiative on Pain Control’s Pain Quality Assessment Scale (PQAS), in which patients are asked to indicate, on a scale of 1 to 10, how "intense" — or "sharp", "hot", "dull", "cold", "sensitive", "tender", "itchy", etc. — their pain has been over the past week."  This isn't quite the 1-10 scale that my physicians have used.  Walsh aptly observes that

the trouble with this approach is the imprecision of that scale of 1 to 10, where a 10 would be "the most intense pain sensation imaginable".  How does a patient ‘imagine’ the worst pain ever and give their own pain a number?  Middle-class British men who have never been in a war zone may find it hard to imagine anything more agonizing than toothache or a tennis injury.  Women who have experienced childbirth may, after that experience, rate everything else as a mild 3 or 4.

            So Walsh talked with a faculty member studying pain; the guy doesn't like numbers.  "There are lots of problems that come with trying to measure pain. . . . I think the obsession with numbers is an oversimplification. Pain is not unidimensional. It doesn’t just come with scale — a lot or a little — it comes with other baggage:  how threatening it is, how emotionally disturbing, how it affects your ability to concentrate."  In the course of a long article, Walsh essentially—whether intentionally or not—demonstrates that there's no reliable and consistent way to measure pain.  This despite tremendous advances in brain research and neuroimaging techniques.

            So what's the state of the world of pain definition?  The most recent update, from the International Association for the Study of Pain (7000 members, clinicians and researchers, in 133 countries) in 2012, defines it as "An unpleasant sensory and emotional experience associated with actual or potential tissue damage, or described in terms of such damage."  The note accompanying the definition amplifies it; here is part of the note.

The inability to communicate verbally does not negate the possibility that an individual is experiencing pain and is in need of appropriate pain-relieving treatment. Pain is always subjective.  Each individual learns the application of the word through experiences related to injury in early life. Biologists recognize that those stimuli which cause pain are liable to damage tissue.  Accordingly, pain is that experience we associate with actual or potential tissue damage. It is unquestionably a sensation in a part or parts of the body, but it is also always unpleasant and therefore also an emotional experience. . . .  Many people report pain in the absence of tissue damage or any likely pathophysiological cause; usually this happens for psychological reasons.  There is usually no way to distinguish their experience from that due to tissue damage if we take the subjective report. If they regard their experience as pain, and if they report it in the same ways as pain caused by tissue damage, it should be accepted as pain.

Wikipedia, always useful for this kind of exploration, helps.  Pain may be a physical phenomenon but emotional state is part of it.  There are situations that had never occurred to me.  If someone suffers pain along with trauma (serious injury or affliction), a recurrence of the pain will typically provoke the original emotional trauma as well.  In the case of two people who receive a similar injury, if one of them associates great emotional stress with it while the other does not, the former will feel more intense pain.  "Modern research has gathered considerable amounts of evidence that support the theory that pain is not only a physical phenomenon but rather a biopsychosocial phenomenon, encompassing culture, nociceptive stimuli, and the environment in the experience and perception of pain."

I would have to delve far more deeply into the literature than I want to in order to discover the most recent views about the measurement of pain in patients.  I'm not quite that interested, but I will be curious to learn what the nurse or physician asks me the next time I'm in their office and I have pain.

Then there's this little research note I came across, which I did not explore further.

Prostaglandins been linked to the sensory perception of pain, but their role in the emotional response to pain is unclear. A new study has demonstrated that the aversive effects of inflammatory pain are driven by prostaglandin signaling specifically on serotonin-producing neurons in the brainstem.

* * *

            I found this an interesting datum.  Researchers (including one from the University of Minnesota) found that "while Americans with higher incomes tend to spend more time alone, [and] when they do socialize, they do so more with their friends.  Meanwhile, the social interactions of those from lower-income households tend to revolve around family members and neighbors."

The implications?  The authors didn’t say so, but a reviewer wondered whether "this dynamic could lead to a decline in civic engagement."  On the one hand, orientation to friends rather than relatives and neighbors could lead people to be less involved in their communities, but there are plenty of other data demonstrating segregation by income in cities, so chances are the friends are also neighbors—or from equally-situated neighborhoods.  The reporter concluded that "at a time when Americans are becoming more economically stratified, it’s worrying that the highest earners might be less likely to have a sense of what’s going on outside of their own peer groups."  I suspect that’s been going on for a long time already.  The people who go to "Friends of the Minnesota Orchestra" events are not typically socializing with the people who frequent soup kitchens.

In our case, I guess we have "higher incomes," at least compared to the average in the U.S., and it’s true that we spend more time with friends than with family—and since our only good friends who were also neighbors moved to Florida, we never spend time with neighbors.  It seems to me they err in suggesting that those with higher incomes might not care about their community.  Some, perhaps, pay no attention to their surrounds, but virtually everyone I know in the Twin Cities—people who are on the liberal end of the political spectrum, it must be acknowledged—is vitally concerned about the entire population, including recent immigrants and those who are not so well off.  The extent to which my affluent friends actually engage with the community varies, of course, but all of them support public policies that extend the social safety net to those around them in our metropolitan area.

* * *

            Sometimes there are academic studies that make my day.  One of them in early 2017 found that there is a link between swearing and honesty.  The study came out of the University of Cambridge in England but the researchers, psychologists, were also from the Netherlands, the U.S., and Hong Kong.  "Frankly, we do give a damn:  The relationship between profanity and honesty."

            Associated with anger or crudeness, often inappropriate, profanity "can have another, more positive connotation. . . .  People who frequently curse are being more honest."  Or, at least, "less likely to be associated with lying and deception."  The article reporting the research defines profanity more broadly than I would, as "language that contains sexual references, blasphemy or other vulgar terms."  I tend to limit profanity to aspects of religion, use vulgarity when the term(s) involves bodily functions and other rude mental images, and apply obscenity to cases that involve sexual activities.  In any case, however defined, it's "usually related to the expression of emotions such as anger, frustration or surprise."  Certainly seems right!

            Both dishonesty and profanity are seen as deviant and "often viewed as evidence of low moral standards."  To the extent that's true in the case of profanity, I know almost no one who has high moral standards—but I don't believe it.  However, on the contrary, the researchers suggest, "profanity can be positively associated with honesty.  It is often used to express unfiltered feelings and sincerity."  Donald Trump used swear words and was seen by some as "more genuine."  I have to think that's right as well.  One of the co-authors said that "the relationship between profanity and dishonesty is a tricky one.  Swearing is often inappropriate but it can also be evidence that someone is telling you their honest opinion.  Just as they aren't filtering their language to be more palatable, they're also not filtering their views."

            To gather data, the researchers first administered a survey to 276 people to ask about the swear words they use and administered lie-detector tests to the subjects to determine if they were being truthful about what they wrote (we'll set aside for now the large methodological problems with lie detectors).  The people who wrote down more swear words were not as likely to be lying about their use.  Then the researchers went to 75,000 U.S. Facebook users and analyzed the use of swear words in online interactions.  "The research found that those who used more profanity were also more likely to use language patterns that have been shown in previous research to be related to honesty."  They also looked at geographic distribution of use of swear words; people in the northeast are more likely to use them than people in the south.  I'm not sure if that surprises me or not.  But if you want to cuss, people will assume you're being honest.

* * *

The original costs of the construction of our house in 1931.  Plumbing at $315 seems right—it's about what I pay every time the plumber shows up.  Unfortunately, the new garage we built a few years ago cost more than $150.  I know that my great aunt and uncle paid $4000 for the house in 1940.  They didn't like having a mortgage, so paid it all off the second or third year.



* * *

Speaking of noses, Cyrano de Bergerac by Edmond Rostand, 1897, Act 1, Scene 4:

CYRANO:
'Tis enormous!
Old Flathead, empty-headed meddler, know
That I am proud possessing such appendice.
'Tis well known, a big nose is indicative
Of a soul affable, and kind, and courteous,
Liberal, brave, just like myself, and such
As you can never dare to dream yourself,
Rascal contemptible!

            Thanks to a friend for pointing this out to me.

No comments:

Post a Comment

Most Read