Tuesday, January 16, 2018

#29 why liberal or conservative, fun responses, happiness & beer, the Queen's power, public health study from 1842, semordnilaps, poem of remembrance




Good morning from continuingly cold Minneapolis.


Why are some people "liberal" and some "conservative"?  That's a question to which there has been much research devoted and there are a multitude of studies that have answers of varying degrees of believability.  One hypothesis is that liberals are more open to ambiguity; another is that family background has a significant effect:  liberals have liberal kids, conservatives have conservative kids.

            I am taken with research out of the University of Illinois and New York University by two psychologists, Larisa Hussak and Andrei Cimpian.  Tom Jacobs reported on their work in Pacific Standard.  The gist of their findings is that if you think external factors determine or strongly affect the person/country/situation—luck, social forces, birth SES— you're more likely to be liberal.  If you think it is internal characteristics—hard work, ability, and so on—then you're more likely to be conservative.  Think about the poor:  "Are they unworthy, or just unlucky?"  When I read about their findings, my gut response was "that seems right."

            Cimpian and Hussak hypothesize that there's a bias in humans about explaining things.  Social phenomena are extremely complicated, so people use explanatory shortcuts, and there's a bias toward attributing status/circumstances  to inherent characteristics rather than social forces.  That bias, in turn, "leads to a tendency to (1) view socioeconomic stratification as acceptable and (2) prefer current societal arrangements to alternative ones, two hallmarks of conservative ideology."  They found these attitudes in children and that they affected later political views.

            Jacobs, in Pacific Standard, observes that previous research suggests that

inherent explanations come to our minds more easily than extrinsic ones. Considering the many external factors that play a role in an individual's success or failure requires considerable cognitive effort.  In contrast, "those people are simply like that" is a simple idea to process—a way to make a reasonable-seeming snap judgment and move on.  If your tendency is to simply go with that initial explanation, you will find yourself in sync with conservative values, including the idea that society is basically fair, and people get what they deserve.

On the other hand, if you believe people's circumstances and actions are shaped by society, family, and luck—external factors—you're less likely to condone economic inequality and will be more likely to support social programs that aid the poor, for example.

I doubt the researchers would maintain that this attribution of causes is the sole explanation for political differences.  It makes sense to me, however, as one significant factor.  Anyone who takes coursework in psychology, sociology, and related fields—and pays attention—will have a hard time relying heavily on internal factors as the primary explanation for where people are in life.  Perhaps that is one reason why the less-well-educated tend to be more conservative and less willing to support social programs (even when those programs support them).  Of course, attitudes beget attitudes in families, so attributing poverty to laziness passes from one generation to the next.

As Jacobs observes, the research suggests an approach "for parents who wish their children to grow up with open minds. . . .  It's easy and natural to blame or celebrate people for their status in life.  The fact that larger, society-level forces play a major role has to be taught."  Yeah, including by going to college, but K-12 education should at the very least make those forces known to students. 

I've tried to remember what kinds of explanations my parents proffered.  I think they used both.  Whatever they said, they didn't freeze my beliefs (and likely didn't intend to), because the more courses in the social and behavioral sciences that I took, the less likely I became to attribute much of an individual's status and circumstances to his or her own effort and worth.  My guess is that parents who embrace inherent characteristics as the cause of status and situation and who are raising children with the same view are not the people who are reading journal research ("Investigating the origins of political views:  Biases in explanation predict conservative attitudes in children and adults." Developmental Science. 2017;e12567. https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12567). 

* * *

            I've had a few amusing responses from friends that made me chuckle (or laugh out loud).

--  "I’m glad to finally have ‘permission’ to quit what needs quitting.  I feel better now.  And looking back, I feel even better!  A good thing."

--  "I totally concur with the Corinthians verse.  Having been raised Catholic, I heard that damn thing all the time and for sure in every Church wedding I ever attended.  It is ridiculous and makes me think:  this is the best they can come up with and that is why it is the only one ever read?"

--  "As a child during and shortly after World War II, spam was often the only meat available, so we ate it fairly often.  It was in fact my favorite meat when I was 5 or 6 years old, and I chose it for a few years as my special birthday dinner.  My mother fried it mostly, but also baked it topped with canned pineapple.  Those were the days! :)"

* * *

            Shifting from solid to liquid intake, research demonstrating that "beer can lift your spirits."  Scientists at a German university looked at 13,000 (!) elements of food to identify those that stimulate the appropriate part of the brain to "make people feel good."  They could do 13,000 because they used computer simulations, not laboratory tests.

Some foods make us happy. Well, maybe not happy but they make us feel good. That is why we cannot stop eating when we have had enough. Scientists call this hedonic hunger -- the drive to eat for pleasure rather than to satisfy an actual biological need.

The reason you feel good from these foods is dopamine, which can be stimulated by certain foods.  One of the substances that stimulates dopamine production—if I understand this process correctly—is something called hordenine.  Beer and malted barley contain hordenine.  So science proves beer makes you happy.  Obviously those who primarily drink wine should consider changing their consumption habits if they want to have a rosier outlook on life.

(https://www.sciencedaily.com/releases/2017/09/170927152838.htm)

* * *

            Seeing the movie "Dunkirk" last year, and then "Darkest Hour" this year, and Kathy watching "The Crown," reminded me of an article I read about the actual authority of the British monarch.  I was startled at how much residual power the Queen has, even though she almost never exercises it.  She doesn't need a passport or a driver's license, but that's not exactly "power."  She also pays taxes voluntarily, although she doesn't have to.

            One of the reasons the British monarchy survived after WWI, according to one historian, is because the monarchs kept their mouths shut.  As we've seen, when they do say things political, it's can be controversial and reduce popular support for the monarchy.  Something I didn't know, although it doesn't surprise me.

For almost two decades now the monarchy has regularly had polls run and focus groups put together to keep track of how the general public feels about them and their various actions.  They also have on payroll individuals whose job it is to ensure the Queen stays in the public eye and in a way that is most likely to endear her to her subjects—as with politicians who rely on the voting public, with each public change she presents, right down to carrying a cell phone or not, carefully calculated in terms of the impact it might have.

            The powers the Queen retains include:

--         declaring war without the approval of Parliament
--         immune from prosecution for any crime
--         combined with diplomatic immunity, she could commit a crime anywhere on Earth and not be prosecuted
--         exempt from requests for information
--         could have anyone arrested and take their property (all are "subjects of the monarch," not citizens of the UK)
--         "owns all of the sea beds around the UK and can commandeer any ship found in British waters 'for service to the realm'"
--         could "administer any manner of punishment to an individual who offended or otherwise displeased her as the crown has 'prerogative power to keep the peace within the realm'"
--         could dissolve Parliament and appoint a prime minister of her choosing no matter the outcome of elections—and keep on calling elections until she gets a Parliament she likes
--         is commander in chief of all British armed forces; every member swears allegiance to the crown
--         must approve laws passed by Parliament before they can go into effect
--         must approve discussion of any bill that "affects the interest of the monarchy" (a power she has used)

As anyone who follows European and British politics knows, the monarch does not rule with the dictatorial authority she theoretically possesses.  I would guess that if she ever did try to flex her muscles in some significant way, that would be the end of the monarchy.  There is a potential benefit to the country for leaving the residual power alone, however.

These powers still exist for a variety of reasons including potentially being needed in a time of extreme crisis where an individual ruling unilaterally for the good of her people can potentially be of benefit—one of the few scenarios her subjects might not mind her flexing her political muscles a bit without necessarily consulting parliament, depending on the circumstances.

            The Queen did exercise her authority to dismiss parliament—the Australian parliament. 

            Some of us, at this point, would welcome the intervention of a crown.

* * *

            This must rank as one of the oldest—if not *the* oldest—public health studies in the western world.  In 1842, a guy named Edwin Chadwick did a study of life expectancy, controlling for geographic area and occupational type; the study covered five urban and rural areas of England.  Three researchers (from the Universities of Liverpool, Oxford, and Glasgow) replicated the study 175 years later.  Here is the table of data from the 1842 Chadwick study:

Table 1.
Average age of death for occupation group by location (after Chadwick, 1842).
Location
Professional Trades
Tradesmen
Labourers
Rutland
52
41
38
Leeds
44
27
19
Liverpool
35
22
15
Manchester
38
20
17
Bolton
34
23
18

            Rutland was a rural area; the others were urban.  My goodness, people did not live long in that era in England; the working conditions for laborers in all the urban areas must have been tough.  That may be an instance where life as hunter-gatherers, before industrial development, would have been longer and more pleasant.  The data are also provocative:  those in the "lowest" occupations in rural Rutland were living longer than those in the "highest" occupations in urban areas (except for Leeds).  However, as one would expect, those in the lower occupational ranks still had higher premature mortality rates than those in higher-status occupations irrespective of geographic area.  What Chadwick demonstrated was the importance of geography and the associated conditions on public health. 

During Chadwick's era, geographic context mattered partly because the cities he studied were, at the extreme, ‘cesspits’ rife with outbreaks of infectious diseases due to insanitary conditions (e.g. Cholera flourished due to a lack of clean water sources or the safe disposal of human waste killing thousands at a time), high level of pollutants due to unregulated industry, and overcrowded slum housing facilitating the spread of diseases.  In contrast, Rutland was an ‘idyllic’ rural settlement set aside from the problems and squalor of Victorian cities.  Much of the field of Health Geography today owes its direction of development and initial impetus to this single piece of evidence.
           
            The modern researchers found that the relationship between geography and health remained.  While the disparities had shrunk, because public health generally has improved dramatically since 1842, there remained differences between the urban and rural groups and between the level of occupation.

Although there is no longer consistent evidence on individuals in the lowest occupational group having lower mortality rates than those in the highest group, there were clear social gradients in mortality within each area and the extent of these inequalities varied between areas.

As one of the study authors put it, "it is remarkable that after 175 years, mortality rates in Liverpool are still higher than in Rutland within each occupational group.  What this demonstrates is that living in certain locations offers very different life chances and health outcomes for people within the same occupational groups."

            To exaggerate, it seems that geography remains destiny.  I'm not sure what to make of the data, but in the U.S. the situation appears to be reversed.  Here's a small excerpt from a news release from the Centers for Disease Control in January of 2017:

Some 46 million Americans — 15 percent of the U.S. population — currently live in rural areas.  Several demographic, environmental, economic, and social factors might put rural residents at higher risk of death from these public health conditions.  Residents of rural areas in the United States tend to be older and sicker than their urban counterparts.  They have higher rates of cigarette smoking, high blood pressure, and obesity.  Rural residents report less leisure-time physical activity and lower seatbelt use than their urban counterparts.  They also have higher rates of poverty, less access to healthcare, and are less likely to have health insurance.

            From Medical Daily:

2005 to 2009, people who resided in large metropolitan areas had a life expectancy of 79.1 years in comparison to [small towns at] 76.9 and [rural areas at] 76.7 years . . . during the same period.  Back in 1969 to 1971, city life expectancy was less than half a year longer than nonmetropolitan areas.  This gap increased to two years between 2005 and 2009.

            The idyllic life in the countryside, at least in the U.S., is also a shorter one.  I wonder to what extent that would change if we had a national health insurance program so all those people in outlying areas were covered.  There would remain the problem of getting to a provider—the simple fact is that there are much longer distances between residents and health care providers in rural areas.

* * *

You may not be aware that the term that appears to be becoming accepted for the words is semordnilap.  A palindrome is a word or phrase that is the same backward or forward; perhaps the most famous is "a man, a plan, a canal, Panama."  The simplest ones are mom, noon, etc.  But what of words, backwards, that are a different word?  One example is evil, another is dog.

The little article that's the source of information about semordnilaps tells me that "one of the earliest direct references to the concept of semordnilaps (though not the name) in English can be found in Lewis Carroll’s 1893 novel Sylvie and Bruno Concluded," where Sylvie puts letters out spelling evil and asks Bruno what they spell; he thinks for a bit and says "live, backwards." 

As for the term itself, it probably originated in 1961 in an annotation by Martin Gardner to the book Oddities and Curiosities of Words and Literature.  "The word is self-referencing in that it demonstrates the concept for which it describes—semordnilap is palindromes spelled backwards."

There are well-known modern examples of semordnilaps.

The mirror Harry stumbles upon in Harry Potter and the Sorcerer’s Stone, which shows a person’s innermost desire is called the Mirror of Erised. Erised is desire spelled backward. In addition, the engraving on the mirror’s frame reads "Erised stra ehru oyt ube cafru oyt on wohsi." If you read this sentence backward and shift the spaces, it reads "I show not your face but your heart’s desire."

Yensid is the name of the sorcerer in Fantasia, which is "Disney" spelled backward.

Harpo is Oprah spelled backward, and is the name of her production company.

            There's your factoid for the day.

* * *

            A friend of mine disputed the sentiment in the quote I had from one of P. D. James's novels about grieving.  ("The tragedy of loss is not that we grieve, but that we cease to grieve, and then perhaps the dead are dead at last.")  She introduced me to a poem by Rabbis Sylvan Kamens and Jack Riemer, We Remember Them, written in 1970 (I think), to be used at shiva or other times of mourning the dead.  It has been adopted by the Unitarians and others for use in services and memorials.

            I have concluded I agree with my friend:  because the grieving stops (or diminishes) does not mean the remembering stops.  The poem is one way to capture the remembrances.  This much better reflects my sentiments.

At the rising of the sun and at its going down
We remember them.
At the blowing of the wind and in the chill of winter
We remember them.
At the opening of the buds and in the rebirth of spring
We remember them.
At the blueness of the skies and in the warmth of summer
We remember them.
At the rustling of the leaves and in the beauty of autumn
We remember them.
At the beginning of the year and when it ends
We remember them.
As long as we live, they too will live;
for they are now a part of us
as we remember them.
When we are weary and in need of strength
We remember them.
When we are lost and sick at heart
We remember them.
When we have joy we crave to share
We remember them.
When we have decisions that are difficult to make
We remember them.
When we have achievements that are based on theirs
We remember them.
As long as we live, they too will live;
for they are now a part of us
as we remember them.


-- Gary


No comments:

Post a Comment

Most Read