Thursday, October 12, 2017

#14 Reflections on high school; high school popularity and later life




            A travelogue from Barcelona-Avignon-Paris will come by and by.  We are in our Paris apartment now; Kathy's having foot problems, so we're keeping it quiet for part of the day today. 

            A few of you have seen this and probably don't want to read it again.  Inasmuch as it consists of a backward look at my high school experiences, many of you might not want to read it.  Whatever.  The second piece is more general.

* * *

There are volumes and volumes of sociological research on American high schools, of which I have read precisely none.  What follows is a reflection on the circumstances of my high school in a particular geographic location at a particular time, uninformed by any research on schools.

Some of the southwestern part of Minneapolis has Minnehaha Creek running through it on a winding east-west path.  My high school, Washburn, sits just north of the creek by a couple of blocks.  The reach of its student body (in the day when the schools drew almost exclusively from the surrounding neighborhoods) included (1) large homes (mostly built before WWII and some before WWI) around or near one of the city lakes (Lake Harriet) and a stretch north of the creek as well as (2) homes that were more modest, primarily post-WWII, and south of the creek.  (This is not a sharp line of demarcation; some of the homes immediately south of the creek were more similar to those north of it than to those farther south.  As with urban development in many cities, there's a hodgepodge of houses both north and south of the creek, but I think my generalization is largely correct.  There is also an area farther north of the school that is also characterized by more modest dwellings, in some cases more modest than those south of the creek.  My focus here, however, is on those in the two areas I have described.) 

When I was at Washburn (1966-69, grades 10-12), it was still a silk-stocking school:  many of the students came from well-heeled backgrounds, from families that resided in the large homes around the east and south side of Lake Harriet and along/north of the creek.  Many others came from the more modest homes south of the creek, and the families by and large had correspondingly more modest incomes.  The latter group, however, was by no means "poor," and in fact was by relative measures affluent, but just not as affluent as those north of the creek.  Washburn, along with Southwest High School, had long been the school for the well-to-do in Minneapolis if the parents didn't send their children to private schools.

            I grew up south of the creek; my elementary and junior high schools were close to our home and, naturally, my friends from grades K-9 came from that area.  When we went to Washburn, we encountered an entirely new group of people who came from a different socio-economic-educational background:  parents who were far more likely to be college-educated and in professional careers, more socially "elite" (I'll leave that term undefined), and who made considerably more money than my family and those of my K-9 circle of friends.  I became friends with some of the kids from that group (and remain friends with a couple of them to this day).  It was clear to me even then, however, that there was a status (I'll leave that term undefined as well) difference between my new high school friends and my friends from earlier grades.

            What happened over my high school years, I realized on reflection a number of years later, is that I gravitated toward the higher-status group and away from my friends from earlier school years.  I can't say I was ever fully a part of the new group; socio-economic class distinctions, even as finely graded as those were—this was a bunch of upper/middle class white kids whose family incomes were different but none of whom were even faintly deprived—are not ever completely erased.  But I did spend much of my time with the new group and far less with the others.  I have reflected from time to time on this evolution and concluded that it was to some extent social climbing and to some extent aspirational:  I wanted to be in an affluent professional class and have friends who were similarly situated.  My parents' friends (even though my father was a college graduate and in a professional career) and my friends' parents—almost all extremely nice people—came predominantly from blue-collar occupations (butcher, carpenter, printer, plumber, electrician).  I liked them but I knew early on that their jobs were not ones that I wanted.

            I met one of the lake/north-of-the-creek group our first quarter in high school, someone with whom I remain good friends.  (I reminded him last fall that we had now known each other for 50 years.  That makes me older than I like but I'm just glad I'm alive to say it.)  It was this friend who brought me into that social circle by including me in events; looking back, I'm not sure why, but he did (I suppose, obviously, because he liked me as a friend).  A couple of other guys from that group also became friends (but, for whatever reason, only one of the girls, whom I dated briefly in college and with whom I also stay in touch).  While I was somewhat close friends with those three or four, I think it was nonetheless true that for the larger group I remained an outsider.  Not disliked, not unwelcome, just not integral.  Had that one friend not begun including me, however, my involvement with that social circle likely never would have happened.

            There is one incident that stands out in my mind that typifies my perception; there were any number of others, but this one I recall vividly.  A group of us (me plus some from the lake/north of the creek area) went to one of the kid's homes after an event.  The girl whose home we went to called her dad about us being there; he adamantly refused to let us stay without one of her parents in the house, so we had to leave.  I recall, however, being impressed by the large entry foyer, the prevalence of dark polished wood everywhere, the broad staircase leading to the second floor, the apparently expensive furnishings and decoration, and the spacious living and dining rooms off the right and left of the foyer.  This was not a house like any of my pre-Washburn friends lived in, but it wasn't out of the ordinary for neighborhood it was in.  (The girl whose house at which this took place confirmed that it was indeed her house, accurately described.)

            At our 45-year class reunion, I was talking with two people (one of whom I had known very well in high school and college, although we drifted apart after that, for no particular reason, and one of whom I had barely known in high school—and still didn't).  My once-upon-a-time friend came from south of the creek, a family of modest means, and the other one from north of the creek (a well-to-do family).  I asked them if they had been aware of this north/south distinction that I had observed; the one from south of the creek immediately nodded his understanding while the one from the affluent family north of the creek said she had no idea what I was talking about.

            I must acknowledge that there's an alternative explanation that may account for some of the difficulty those of us from south of the creek had in joining the lake/north-of-the-creek social circles.  Ramsey Junior High School sits on the same city superblock as Washburn; the two are divided by the Washburn football field and bleachers.  A huge percentage of Ramsey students moved to Washburn, friendships and social circles intact.  That group included the students who came from the lake/north-of-the-creek area.  Only about half of the students from my junior high school, Anthony, came to Washburn, so a number of those junior high school social circles were disrupted.  It may be that those of us from Anthony were not able easily to join the established social circles (and pecking order) that had transferred mostly intact from Ramsey.

            It may also be true that this distinction between students from south of the creek and the lake/north of the creek is a figment of my imagination.  My friend's instant recognition of it, however, suggests the perception is not uniquely mine.  It is possible, however, that by and large those from the lake/north-of-the-creek area did not perceive the distinction, perhaps partly because they had their established (affluent) social circles and weren't paying much attention to other people.  It is also possible that many from south of the creek didn't notice the distinction, either, for the same reason—they had social circles that had survived the transition to Washburn—or they noticed but didn't care.

It would be an omission worthy of criticism were I not to include an assessment of my personal characteristics (or my perception of them, anyway), which certainly could have affected my reception by new groups of classmates.  I don't think I was in the "egghead" category, or whatever it was, and I surely wasn't in the "popular" category.  Like the majority of students, I was just there, neither outstanding or attractive in any particular fashion nor particularly repellant or different, neither reclusive nor outgoing, certainly not "hail fellow well met."  As I look back on myself, I think "bland" might be one appropriate term, and thus it is perhaps less surprising that people did not immediately jump to welcome me as a new friend.  I don't think that anyone in my professional career or my current circle of friends would use that term.

            By the time we were all off to college (at that time something like 90+% of Washburn graduates went to college), my path parted even more from my friends from my youngest years—but never completely.  As is true for everyone who goes to college, I made entirely new friends.  In my experience it is the college and graduate/professional school friends as well as those made during one's career who tend to remain lifelong friends, as opposed to friends from K-12 school years.

            So where do things stand after 50 years?  Of the lake/north-of-the-creek circle, I remain good friends with two of the guys (and, as I mentioned, with one of the girls).  Two other guys vanished from my life shortly after college (one of them vanished from the life of everyone who'd been at Washburn, as far as any of us can tell).  Inasmuch as I never developed any solid friendships with the rest of them, they faded away the same way that many of one's high school classmates do.  At class reunions, I have conversations with many; to whatever extent my characterization of the distinctions between groups of students is accurate, it has mostly disappeared (although, naturally, people who knew each other well in high school still tend to congregate even if they only see each other at the reunions).

All of the foregoing being said, I never lost touch with the friends who'd been close in elementary and junior high school, but I didn't see them more than once every 5-6 years.

            Until recently.  I've been glad in the last year or so to resume a more frequent friendship with a couple of my friends from my youngest days.  I'm glad they did not, at some point, say or think "to hell with him" because of the distance I let grow between us.  It's been rewarding to renew and refresh the relationships.  We are all fairly recently retired, from professional careers, so even though we were south-of-the-creek guys, we went into the professions, not the trades.  We came of age, and in a high school, where the children of white middle-class parents were going to college—even if their parents hadn't.  So as we wander towards our senescence, we find we have much in common.

            So what's my overall take on my high school experience five decades later?  I'm ambivalent.  Socially it was a confusing time for me, somewhat remote from my friends from elementary and junior high school but never more than a hanger-on (at best) of the aspirational group I encountered at Washburn.  Academically, it was extraordinarily rewarding:  we had a crackerjack philosophy teacher, I think my history teachers are responsible for my life-long avocational reading in history, I enjoyed the social science classes, and I'll grudgingly concede that I learned Shakespeare and some literature from my Washburn teachers.  Math and the sciences?  Not so much, foreshadowing a lifelong lack of interest in the fields (except, in recent decades, as an avid lay reader of advances in the sciences).  I don't have any truly bad recollections of high school, just some that were frustrating.  To the extent that I "blossomed," it was in college and thereafter, perhaps evidenced by the fact that with only one short interruption I remained at the University of Minnesota for 46 years as a student and staff member and achieved, some faculty and administrators have told me, minor "iconic" status for the work I did.  I can settle for that.

One thing I want to be careful about is implying that the more affluent classmates looked "down" on those of us from homes that were less affluent than some north of the creek; I don't believe that at all.  My point was that the difference existed, some were indifferent to it, some didn't notice it, and some just didn't pay attention because they already had a social group coming out of Ramsey.

[I don't touch on the issue of race in this reflection because there were very few students of color at Washburn in the late 1960s.  Of our graduating class of ~750, perhaps 20-30 were non-white (I haven't counted the pictures in the yearbook; maybe it was more—but if so, not a lot).  I can recall encountering only one or two non-white students in elementary and junior high school (and they were Asian-Americans); the neighborhoods, and thus the schools, were almost entirely white.  I'm not claiming race and school segregation wasn't an issue in Minneapolis schools, only that it isn't germane to the points I wish to make here.]

            A note on terminology.  I have used the terms "guys" and "girls" in the preceding narrative.  It seemed to me just wrong to use "men" and "women" in this context; I don't believe we thought of ourselves as "men" and "women" at the time except in a purely biological sense.  I also tried out using "boys" and "girls," but that didn't fit, either.  I came to realize that at least in my mind, using "guys" and "girls" together implies teenagers or very young adults, whereas using "boys" and "girls" together implies little kids.  (And using "guys" and "gals" together in a text would imply adults.)  This nuance may be entirely my imagination, but that's my story and I'm sticking to it.  I consulted with my long-time faculty friend in English to whom I turn whenever I have questions like this; he told me, apropos of guys and gals and boys and girls, that "the distinctions are real but unfortunately not fixed, and the context will often imply the right application."  My interpretation of his comment is that I can use the terms as I have within the narrow context of a high school setting.  My use of "girls" in this part of the letter obviously should not be construed as demeaning females/women.  See my note about Christine Grant.

* * *

            I suppose I shouldn't be surprised that when I write a few paragraphs on a subject, I notice over the following few weeks or months that an article appears in the scholarly or popular press that's directly related to what I wrote.  It happened with high school popularity.

            Mitch Prinstein, a University of North Carolina distinguished-chair psychology professor, has published a book on popularity cleverly titled Popular.  (He also teaches enormously popular courses on the subject).  Popularity, so the thinking goes, was something we left behind when we departed high school for college and careers and became grown-ups.  Prinstein (reviewed in the Atlantic) says that's not the case.  "'In a very real manner . . . our experiences with popularity are always occupying our minds.'  He adds:  'We never really left high school at all.'"  He also wrote that "our high school selves are 'deeply embedded in our souls forever.'"  Given my perception of my high school self, this proposition makes me shudder.

            In Prinstein's view, there are two bases for popularity:  status and likeability (about one-third of those with high status are also likeable).  Status popularity is high school stuff we can all recall; it may provoke admiration but not necessarily liking.  Likeability is just that—related "to charm, to friendliness, to inquisitiveness—it’s the charisma that draws other people to you, largely independent of status or beauty or any of the other metrics that generally give people rank in American culture."

            Moreover, for neurological reasons related to the enormous growth of the human brain around the time of puberty (and, so, the time of middle/high school), "newfound brain capacity collides with newfound self-consciousness.  The adolescent brain is primed both to take in the world around it more than ever before, and to process that information with more self-awareness than ever before.  Which is another way of saying that teenagers are particularly cognizant of identity."  High school, so the research suggests, is "a powder keg, emotionally, and popularity—or, more specifically, teens’ conception of popularity—is a fuse."

Prinstein also makes a strong case for everlasting adolescence:  He cites study after study . . . all suggesting the ways that popularity imprints itself on people’s lives, far beyond the teenage years, through both its presence and its absence.  Popularity affects people’s ability to find success in their careers, regardless of their intelligence or their work ethic.  It affects their ability to find fulfilling friendships and romantic relationships.

            I don't have the means to conduct an analysis of my high school class of 750, but my impression from communicating with many of my classmates suggests that the most popular people in high school are not necessarily the ones who have been most successful in life (defining "success" in some general fashion).  I have some trouble buying into all this.

            What is probably unarguable is that popularity (defined as social connections) is linked to health and lifespan.  There's ample research demonstrating that the larger the network of friends, the longer the life, and the higher the quality of the relationships, the greater the link between them and longevity.  The converse was also true, not surprisingly:  those with fewest social connections had the higher mortality rates ("being unpopular . . . increased subjects' chances of death—more strongly, in fact, than did obesity, physical inactivity, or binge-drinking.  (The only factor that seemed to be comparably hazardous to subjects' longevity?  Smoking.)")  Lack of popularity "has also been one of the most consistent risk factors for depression, anxiety, substance abuse, and even criminal behavior."

So, the theory goes,

even forty years later, we can predict who will graduate from high school or college, who will succeed at work, who will apply for welfare/social services, and who may suffer from debilitating mental health difficulties or addictions all by knowing how popular folks were in high school.  What may be most surprising, however, is that our popularity plays a role that cannot be accounted for by our socioeconomic status, IQ, family background, prior mental health difficulties, or our appearance.  There’s something about the way we are regarded by others that changes our life trajectories quite meaningfully and substantially.

Our biases grow out of our high school experiences and popularity.  We are suspicious or trusting, we assume friendliness or conflict.  In Prinstein's words, "Our adult brains began to form to help us survive in the hallways of high school. The problem is, we left high school long ago—and our brains never got the memo."

            I find this depressing, if it's true. 

No comments:

Post a Comment

Most Read